Could there
really be pets that understand everything we say? The dream of speaking to
animals runs from Sigurd in Norse mythology right up to Dr Dolittle. Some
researchers have claimed that they can actually do it, by teaching their subjects
sign language or another mutually comprehensible means of communication.
These included famous chimps like Nim and Washoe and the gorilla Koko. A few years ago, we were on the brink of a breakthrough in inter-species communications – so what happened?
These included famous chimps like Nim and Washoe and the gorilla Koko. A few years ago, we were on the brink of a breakthrough in inter-species communications – so what happened?
Wittgenstein
famously commented that if a lion could talk, we would not be able to
understand it.[1] He believed that different ‘forms of life’ would have such
different viewpoints that there could not be any common ground. We might
exchange phrases like “there are two zebras over there”, but cannot hope to
understand lion ethics or æsthetics, or even know what ‘zebra’ really means to
a lion. Think how hard it is for non-enthusiasts to ‘get’ a football fan’s
enthusiasm and multiply by a million.
A few years
later, the linguist Noam Chomsky suggested that language is an innate human
skill and that we share an in-built ‘universal grammar’ common to all human
languages. Animals, lacking this grammar, would not be able to communicate in
the same way. By way of challenge, in 1967 Allen and Beatrix Gardner set about
teaching a chimpanzee called Washoe to talk.
Washoe was
given a highly stimulating environment, with toys, games, books and friends to
play with. In many ways, she was treated like a human child, and had four main
human companions who stayed with her for many years. Originally based in Washoe
County, Nevada, she was later moved to the Central University of Washington.
Washoe’s
carers communicated with each other in sign language, and she gradually picked
it up herself, learning over 350 words. There are many anecdotal accounts of
her language skill; she made up her own combination words, such as using the
signs for “water” and “bird” when she saw a swan. Washoe lived until 2007, but
research had moved on.
A second
project in 1973 called “Nim Chimpsky” aimed at a more rigorous scientific
approach. Like Washoe, Nim learned a significant vocabulary in sign language
and could, for example, point to or ask for a banana or yoghurt. Meanwhile, in
Project Koko, a lowland gorilla learned over a thousand words and is also
credited with combining ‘water and ‘bird’ to describe a duck landing on a lake.
These apes also proved capable of teaching others sign language and using it
among themselves.
Critics objected
that this was simple signing, and that neither Nim nor Washoe had any grasp of
grammar in spite of the researchers’ claims. It looked more like conditioning,
learning that a certain action brings a reward without any deeper
understanding. And a closer look at film of the chimps producing what might be
taken as intelligent communication showed something else: “The frame-by-frame
analysis revealed trainers unconsciously prompting and modelling each word, and
Nim imitating. Unaware of their own prompting, the trainers had credited Nim
with producing sentences.”[2]
Clever Hans, the 19th-century German horse and calculating prodigy, lost his calculating powers when there was nobody around for him to pick up cues from. Sebeok suggested that similar effects were at work in the ape communications field, and that there was self-deception among researchers over just how well their subjects were communicating.
“Give
orange me give eat orange me eat orange give me eat orange give me you,” was
reportedly Nim’s longest utterance. This certainly communicates something, and
might work as a lyric, but it does look like signalling rather than language.
While the
believers kept on believing – as always – the mainstream view moved sharply
away. There was too much suspicion that researchers were simply seeing what
they wanted to see.
Ape
intelligence studies continue, such as the one involving Kanzi, a chimp who can
make and use tools and communicate via a keyboard.[3] He’s a remarkable chimp,
but there are few who would claim that Kanzi can form grammatical sentences.
“Get red ball,” “ball get red” and “red ball get” are all the same.
Meanwhile,
there is also work with other species. Forgetting questionable efforts such as
the Bowlingual translator (which ‘translates’ dog barks into human language),
there is the Cetacean Hearing and Telemetry (CHAT) programme. Denise Herzing of
the Wild Dolphin Project in Florida is working on a device for divers to talk
to dolphins in something resembling dolphinese. The device will be able to
detect and analyse dolphin squeaks and generate noises in reply. The idea is
that a diver plays a given sound which the dolphin will associate with an
action like “play with seaweed”, and that the dolphin will learn to use the
sound itself.[4]
CHAT builds
on previous dolphin efforts using underwater keyboards, and may one day be very
useful for practical dolphin-human communications on the level of “get red
ball”. But however smart dolphins may be, there’s little chance of a real
conversation yet. As Hans and Nim and the others have shown, the risk is that
when we think we’re talking to them, they’re just reflecting us and we’re
really talking to ourselves.
NOTES
1 Ludwig
Wittgenstein: Philosophical Investigations (1953).
2 The
Animal Communication Project.
3 Great Ape
Trust.
4 New Scientist, 9 May 2011.
Δεν υπάρχουν σχόλια:
Δημοσίευση σχολίου