Yogi Berra, the former Major League baseball catcher and
coach, once remarked that you can’t hit and think at the same time.
Of course,
since he also reportedly said, “I really didn’t say everything I said,” it is
not clear we should take his statements at face value. Nonetheless, a
widespread view — in both academic journals and the popular press — is that
thinking about what you are doing, as you are doing it, interferes with
performance. The idea is that once you have developed the ability to play an
arpeggio on the piano, putt a golf ball or parallel park, attention to what you
are doing leads to inaccuracies, blunders and sometimes even utter paralysis.
As the great choreographer George Balanchine would say to his dancers, “Don’t
think, dear; just do.”
We should conclude that poor choices come not from thinking
but from not being trained how to think.
Perhaps you have experienced this destructive force
yourself. Start thinking about just how to carry a full glass of water without
spilling, and you’ll end up drenched. How, exactly, do you initiate a telephone
conversation? Begin wondering, and before long, the recipient of your call will
notice the heavy breathing and hang up. Our actions, the French philosopher
Maurice Merleau-Ponty tells us, exhibit a “magical” efficacy; yet when we focus
on them, they degenerate into the absurd. A 13-time winner on the Professional
Golfers Association Tour, Dave Hill, put it like this: “Golf is like sex. You
can’t be thinking about the mechanics of the act while you are performing.”
But why not?
A classic study by Timothy Wilson and Jonathan Schooler is
frequently cited in support of the notion that experts, when performing at
their best, act intuitively and automatically and don’t think about what they
are doing as they are doing it, but just do it. The study divided subjects, who
were college students, into two groups. In both groups, participants were asked
to rank five brands of jam from best to worst. In one group they were asked to
also explain their reasons for their rankings. The group whose sole task was to
rank the jams ended up with fairly consistent judgments both among themselves
and in comparison with the judgments of expert food tasters, as recorded in
Consumer Reports. The rankings of the other group, however, went haywire, with
subjects’ preferences neither in line with one another’s nor in line with the
preferences of the experts. Why should this be? The researchers posit that when
subjects explained their choices, they thought more about them.
Tucker Nichols
The expert food tasters were able to both provide reasons
for their choices and, arguably, make the best choices. Thus, we should conclude that poor choices
come not from thinking but from not being trained how to think.
Although novice athletes need to think about what they are
doing, experts in normal situations, we are told by the University of Chicago
psychologist Sian Beilock in her recent book “Choke,” ought not to since, as
she puts it, “careful consideration can get them in trouble.” Based on
experiments she and her colleagues have performed, she concludes that
high-pressure situations lead experts to think in action and that such thinking
tends to cause choking, or “paralysis by analysis.” To prevent this, she
advises, you need to “play outside your head.”
Yet contrary to Beilock’s findings, rather than keeping
their minds out of the picture, experts at least sometimes seem to avoid the
type of performance detriments associated with high-pressure situations
precisely by concentrating intensely and focusing on the details of their
movements. When one’s mind is consumed
by the angular momentum of one’s golf swing, there is no room for the type of
nerves that lead to a choke.
That experts increase their focus on the task at hand in
order to cope with pressure is suggested by studies, like those carried out by
the University of Hull sports and exercise psychologist Adam Nicholls, which
ask professional or elite athletes to keep a diary of stressors that occur and
coping strategies that they employ during games. Though small scale, these
studies do indicate that a common method of dealing with stress involves
redoubling both effort and attention. As Nicholls told me, when I asked him
about it, “increasing effort was an effective strategy and really helped the
players.”
Of course, one may wonder whether athletes, or anyone for
that matter, have accurate insight into what goes on in their minds. Beilock’s work, for the most part, avoids
this worry since her methods are comparatively objective. For example, in one
study she and her research team measured the accuracy of golf putts when
players were distracted from what they are doing compared with when they were
asked to focus on what they are doing.
Such a procedure does not rely on subjective reports.
Yet there is a trade-off between greater objectivity and
what researchers call “ecological validity.” Nicholls’s work, while perhaps
less objective, is more ecologically valid because it looks at experts in
real-life settings, asking them to do nothing other than what they would
normally do. In contrast, Beilock — in a study suggesting that “well-learned
performance may actually be compromised by attending to skill execution” — asks
subjects who are putting golf balls to say “stop,” out loud at the exact moment
they finish the follow-through of their swing. True enough, this interfered
with performance. Yet expert golfers do not normally focus on the exact moment
they complete their follow-through; expert golfers, when thinking about their
actions, focus on something that matters. In fact, such a task would seem to be
more distracting than any of the study’s explicit distractions.
Moreover, in contrast to Beilock’s studies, which typically
use college students as subjects, Nicholls works with professional-level
experts. This is relevant since, as with Wilson and Schooler’s experiment, it
is the expert and not necessarily the college student with a couple of years,
or sometimes a couple of hours, of experience who has the ability to hit and
think at the same time.
Though the University of California at Berkeley philosopher
Hubert Dreyfus takes his inspiration more from Merleau-Ponty and Martin
Heidegger than from empirical studies, the conclusions he arrives at resonate
with Beilock’s. Dreyfus has long argued that “the enemy of expertise is
thought” and that the apogee of human performance is exemplified in seamless,
unreflective actions in which the self disappears. In a debate with the University of Pittsburgh
philosopher John McDowell, published in the journal Inquiry, Dreyfus tells us
that whenever Homer describes his heroes at a feast, instead of having them
deliberately reach for bread in baskets or bowls brimful to drink, “their arms
shot out to the food lying ready before them.” Similarly, says Dreyfus, the
grandmaster chess player might find “his arm going out and making a move before
he can take in the board position.” As with the master archer in Eugen
Herrigel’s perennially popular “Zen in the Art of Archery,” neither Odysseus
feasting at a banquet nor the grandmaster playing chess moves his arm; rather
“it shoots.”
RELATED
More From The Stone
Read previous contributions to this series.
It may very well be that our ordinary actions, like eating —
especially when famished after a battle — do in some sense just happen to us.
Yet what are we to say about the grandmaster as he closes the mating net?
According to Dreyfus, Merleau-Ponty and Heidegger teach us that “what we are
directly open to is not rational or even conceptual … [rather,] it is the
affordance’s solicitation — such as the attraction of an apple when I’m
hungry.” The problem with this picture for chess, however, is that the
attractive apple is often poisoned. In such cases, leaving reason behind leads
you right into your opponent’s trap.
In “All Things Shining: Reading the Western Classics to Find
Meaning in a Secular Age,” which can be seen as a paean to the idea that
exemplary performance happens to, rather than is done by, an individual,
Dreyfus and his co-author, the Harvard philosopher Sean Kelly, argue that
letting the self get washed away in action is the key to living a meaningful
life: it is when we are “taken over by the situation” that life “really shines
and matters most.” But is this right?
The question “what is the meaning of life?” is, of course, a big one; however,
if it includes developing one’s potential, what Immanuel Kant spoke of in the
“Groundwork for the Metaphysics of Morals” as our duty to cultivate our
“predispositions to greater perfection,” a duty he saw as often involving a
struggle, then the idea that the expert should stand back and effortlessly let
it happen just isn’t going to do it.
I remember how difficult philosophy seemed when I was an
undergraduate at Berkeley. I was taking epistemology with Barry Stroud at the
time and, feeling a bit disheartened, went to his office and asked, “Does it
ever get easier?” No, it doesn’t, he
told me, since as you grow as a philosopher you work on increasingly more
difficult problems. Although this was not the response I was hoping for, it
made sense immediately, for I was entering college directly from a career as a
professional ballet dancer. Ballet, I
knew, never gets easier; if anything, it gets harder, because as you develop as
a dancer you develop both stricter standards for what counts as good dancing
and your ability to evaluate your dancing, finding flaws that previously went
unnoticed. Just as in Plato’s dialogue, the Apology, Socrates is wise because
he knows that he is ignorant, it is, among other things, the ability to
recognize where there is room for improvement that allows expert dancers to
reach great heights.
The ability to see room for improvement, however, is not of
much use unless one also has a strong and continuing desire to improve. And it
may be that, more so than talent, it is this desire to improve, an attitude the
Japanese call “kaizen,” that turns a novice into an expert. I certainly had kaizen in abundance, as did
most every professional dancer I knew.
It was ingrained in my body and mind to the extent that every class,
rehearsal and performance was in part aimed at self-improvement. And improving, especially after you have
acquired a high level of skill, typically requires an enormous amount of
effort. Sometimes this effort is
physical — and it certainly involves more physical effort than philosophy — yet
it also involves concentration, thought, deliberation and will power.
The philosophers and psychologists who advocate a just-do-it
mentality all admit that during those rare occasions when something goes wrong,
performers or athletes need to direct their attention to their actions. Yet
although from an audience’s point of view, things rarely go wrong, from the
expert’s point of view, things are going wrong all the time. Lynn Seymour, who
was a principal dancer with the Royal Ballet, commented that when she danced
with Rudolph Nureyev in the film “I Am a Dancer,” she was too cowardly to ever
watch it: “whenever I see myself dancing I practically die.”
When we make the morning coffee, tie our shoes or drive to
work, we are satisfied with performance that is good enough. And it is easy to
see how an evolutionary advantage could accrue to those who could think about
more important things during routine activities like grooming. Yet there are
significant differences between everyday actions and the actions of experts,
since for a golfer at the U.S. Open or a dancer on stage at the Royal Opera
House, there is nothing more important than the task at hand.
Perhaps golf is like sex, not because, as Dave Hill claimed,
attention to performance interferes with expert action, but rather because both
the sex drive and the expert’s drive to excel can be all-encompassing. And
excelling in such highly competitive arenas as professional-level golf requires
not just doing what has normally worked but doing better than ever. Yet doing
better than ever cannot be automatic.
In its “just-do-it” advertising campaign, Nike presumably
used the phrase to mean something like, “stop procrastinating, get off your
posterior and get the job done.” Interpreted as such, I’m in favor of
“just-do-it.” However, when interpreted as “experts perform best when not
thinking about what they are doing,” the idea of just-do-it is a myth.
If so, the enormous popularity of books that tell us how to
achieve mastery in chess, cinch a business deal or become a better parent with
neither effort nor thought nor attention may turn on our preference for
“magical” efficiency over honest toil. They reach the status of best sellers
for the same reason as do diet books that advocate eating as much as you want
as long as you abstain from some arbitrary category of food: not because they
work, but because they are easy to follow.
As for
Balanchine’s claim that his dancers shouldn’t think, I asked Violette Verdy
about this. Verdy was a principal dancer with New York City Ballet for 18 years
under Balanchine’s direction. But she brushed off the question. “Oh,
that,” she replied. “He only said that when a dancer was stuck; like an
elevator between floors.”
By BARBARA GAIL MONTERO
Δεν υπάρχουν σχόλια:
Δημοσίευση σχολίου